Saturday, June 5, 2021

 

Chapter 3: Research Method

The problem addressed in this study was a lack of immigrant and refugee parent involvement in Head Start and Early Head Start programs to support their children’s education (Hindman et al., 2012; Manz et al., 2014). Specifically, this study addressed the lack of knowledge regarding what variables relate to high and/or low parent involvement and identification of those that served as barriers to or promoters of parent involvement in this population. Some of the barriers for immigrant and refugee families were likely their pre-conceived beliefs about education (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013) as well as differences due to language and culture (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013). The variables, as defined within the theory of planned behavior (TPB) that contribute to parental intentions and were included in the study attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control (Ajzen, 1991; Bracke & Corts, 2012; Perry & Langley, 2013). These variables had not been thoroughly examined in the context of immigrant and refugee parents such that this knowledge could be applied to help devise parental interventions for these parents (McGregor & Knoll, 2015) in order to improve parental intention for involvement in their children’s formal schooling. Without further understanding of how these variables relate to and predict parental intentions for involvement, strategies employed by these programs to increase the parental involvement of immigrant and refugee families may be less effective and the families may not take full advantage of these programs (Lee & Zhou, 2014). Identifying determinants of parent involvement, or lack thereof, in immigrant and refugee populations could subsequently be used to develop interventions (Hindman et al., 2012).

The purpose of this quantitative, descriptive and correlational study was to assess key variables, posed in the TPB as possible determinants of parent intention for school involvement behavior (i.e., parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and ascertain whether they were significantly related to and could predict the reported intentions of involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in their children’s early childhood education programs. By assessing the constructs that were pivotal to TPB, an expansion of this theory within the context of immigrant and refugee families with children enrolled in Head Start or Early Head Start programs was fulfilled. Using a survey instrument to collect data, the goals included the following: (1) identify parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions regarding parent involvement, and (2) investigate how, if at all, these variables correlated with and served to predict parent involvement, as TPB would suggest, in this population. The dependent variable was parental intentions for involvement, and the predictors were the determinants of behaviors as outlined in TPB (i.e., parent attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) as reported by immigrant and refugee families with children enrolled in Head Start and Early Head Start programs.

The target population was 800-1000 parents/caregivers who were foreign born, i.e., first-generation immigrants or refugees (Krogstad, 2015; Winsler et al., 2014), were living in Southern California, and had children enrolled in either an Early Head Start or Head Start program. A census of this population was conducted with the goal of obtaining a sample of 122 parents/caregivers whose children participated in these programs. A power analysis using G-Power software yielded an estimated sample size of 110 for a linear regression with three predictors (power of 0.8, type one error of 0.05, and medium size effect) (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009). Parents were asked to complete the Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire (Appendix A). It was a 57-item survey that had been found reliable and valid for measuring attitudes and beliefs (24 items), subjective norms (6 items), perceived behavioral control (17 items), and parental intentions (10 items). All items were evaluated using six-point Likert scales. Data collection provided an opportunity to describe how these variables presented in immigrant and refugee families and examined their ability to predict parent intentions towards involvement using the three predictor variables of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control. The findings serve to further inform the TPB about its applicability to immigrant and refugee parents who had children attending one of the Head Start or Early Head Start programs. Step-wise multiple regressions were used to assess the significance of the contributions of each predictor to explain the variation of the dependent variable (Field, 2013). Survey methods were employed, as they were the most appropriate method for collecting quantitative data to measure study variables in the TPB model to ascertain whether they were significantly related to and ccould predict the involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in their children’s early childhood education programs (Girardelli & Patel, 2016). This chapter includes discussion of the research design, population/sample, materials/instrumentations, operational definations of variables, study procedures, data collection and analysis, assumptions, limitations, delimitations, ethical assurances, and ethical guarantees that apply in the proposed study.

 

Research Design

A quantitative, descriptive and correlational design was employed to answer the research questions. A quantitative design was the most appropriate method since it generated numerical data that were analyzed to determine relationships between the multiple predictor variables in TPB (Park & Park, 2016). A regression model was used to investigate whether the TPB determinants of behavior were significantly related to and predicted the parental intentions for involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in their children’s early childhood education programs. The descriptive and correlational design allowed for assessing the key variables in TPB such as the determinants of behavior (i.e., parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions) and determining whether they were significantly related to and predicted the intentions of immigrant and refugee parents for being involved in their children’s early childhood education programs. The approach employed in this study was an appropriate technique for testing whether TPB constructs fit a model for an observed set of correlations among the constructs in the model (Girardelli & Patel, 2016).

A cross-sectional design was used to collect data to assess variables in TPB gathered at a single point in time using valid and reliable instruments (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). A cross-sectional survey required less dedication from research participants, took less time to complete, and did not contain many obstacles related to finding and maintaining a sample population (Leedy & Ormrod, 2010) as opposed to a longitudinal study that must take place across at least two waves of times (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). This method of data collection was equated with surveys that must be carefully designed prior to the research occurring (Fowler, 2009). This design allowed collection of data that could measure variables quantitatively for statistical analyses from a sample that could be generalized to a target population, while remaining objective, separated from the subject matter, unbiased, and value-free (Smith, 2015). The correlational research design sought to control preferences, so those facts, instances, and phenomena were understood in an objective way (Park & Park, 2016). The strength of this research design was in the information that could be reported in the form of numbers and could test a formulated hypothesis prior to the actual collection of data (McCusker & Gunaydin, 2014). McCusker and Gunaydin pointed out that when using this design, the extraction of information in a larger volume and emphasis on statistical information rather than individual perceptions. The weakness of the survey method occurs when the answers from sampled respondents are not accurately measurable and become erroneous. To avoid this problem, a reliable and valid survey was used (Fowler, 2009). A survey designed by Hoover-Dempsey, Walker, Jones, and Reed (2002) was employed. The validity and reliability of the survey were previously established in studies by Hoover-Dempsey, Walkers, and Sandler (2002; 2005). The survey consisted of 57 questions based on a six-point Likert scale assessed the following variables: attitudes/beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions. The survey takes approximately 30 minutes to complete and was returned to the researcher in person or by mail to receive a gift card. By using a survey design, there were some advantages such as inexpensive unit costs (Fowler, 2009); however, some disadvantages included that parents did not always submit the surveys via mail within the timeframe given (Fowler, 2009). The following section describes how the sample was obtained in this study.

 

Population and Sample

The population for this study included all immigrant and refugee parents or caregivers from All Kids Academy Head Start and Early Head Start programs (AKA-HS/EHS). According to AKA HS/EHS’s 2015 annual reported, a subgroup of immigrant and refugee parents or caregivers with ethnic and racial diversity for potential generalizability was identified (Hindman, Miller, Froyen, & Skibbe, 2012). In 2015, AKA Head Start programs served 1,167 children from low-income families. According to the Poverty Guidelines published by a federal government, they were eligible for HS and EHS services. All AKA Head Start centers provided full day and part day child care services to families year-round for 10.5 hours per day, Monday through Friday from 7:00 am to 5:30 am, and 3.5 hours per day, Monday through Thursday from 8:15 am to 11:45 am or 1:15 pm to 4:45 pm. Of the enrolled children from 2 to 5 years, 44% of the AKA HS and EHS populations were immigrant and refugee families consisting of the following demographics: Spanish (34%), Middle Eastern and South Asian (8%), East Asian (1%), and African (1%). The AKA Early Head Start program serves 214 children under three years of age of which 43% were identified as immigrant or refugees—who were Spanish (37%), Middle Eastern and South Asian (5%), and East Asian (1%) (AKA Head Start/Early Head Start Annual Report 2014-2015; Appendix J).

    A target of 800 to 1000 parents or caregivers who potentially met the participant criteria were solicited to participate in the survey with the hopes of obtaining the sample size requirement of 110 completed surveys. To participate, a parent or caregiver had to be first-generation, foreign born, and considered an immigrant or refugee. They must have been living in Southern California and have children enrolled in either an Early Head Start or Head Start program. A power analysis yielded an estimated sample size of 110 for a linear regression with four predictors (power of 0.8, type I error of 0.05, and medium size effect of 0.1) (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009); however, all attempts were made to obtain the largest possible sample.

This study used a censuses sampling procedure by attempting to reach all parents that met the criteria for inclusion in the study. The sample was readily available to obtain in person at the 12 sites of AKA HS/EHS. Individuals were self-selected into the sample by choosing to complete the survey (Fowler, 2009). The researcher asked the AKA Head Start executive director and center directors to collaborate with the project to ensure all immigrant/refugee parents/caregivers had a chance to participate. In this way, the researcher avoided bias that could affects the relationship between a sample of respondents and the population (Fowler, 2009). To avoid a problem of bias, the researcher attempted to gather data from every member of the sample population or sampling frame as the surveys were distributed at a center’s gate where parents/caregivers would drop-off their children.

Materials/Instrumentation

A self-administered paper-based survey method was used for several reasons. First, the sample in this population may not have had a computer or Internet access on a regular basis to do an email survey. Second, the researcher was able to identify and access the sample population with relative ease in person at AKA Head Start centers. Third, the participants, the parents or caregivers, could read and interpret survey questions in their own language and then answer with restricted selection options, such as circling a number or checking a mark, which eliminated the need for someone else to read the questions for respondents (Fowler, 2009). Fourth, parents or caregivers were likely to cooperate with the researcher by presenting their perceptions about parental involvement using this method. Finally, some parents or caregivers were usually busy working and had no time to do a survey or had a little time to be at home with their child. This self-administered survey allowed them to complete the survey at their own convenience (Fowler, 2009). The method of the self-administered survey was most appropriate for this study; however, there were some disadvantages to this approach. First, printing hundreds of paper surveys were cost the researcher along with postages and self-addressed stamped envelopes. Second, returning the surveys from respondents took a longer time than the three to four weeks expected (Fowler, 2009). The researcher also needed additional time to enter the responses into an electronic format (Blaxter, Hughes, & Tight, 2014), in this case the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 22.0. Furthermore, tracking completed surveys and incentives cost the researcher’s time (i.e., creating an Excel worksheet listing potential respondents who completed the survey and whether an incentive was provided to them) (Survey Administration Guidelines, 2009).

The Parent Involvement Project (PIP) survey (Appendix A) was the instrument used for data collection. Permission was obtained prior to using this instrument (Appendix I). The questionnaire contains 57 items developed by Hoover-Dempsey, Walker, Jones, and Reed (2002). All items were measured on a six-point Likert scale with subscales of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions for involvement. The Likert response scale ranges from (1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree, with the additional option of I Don’t Know. The options were: 1 indicating Strongly Disagree, 2 indicating Disagree, 3 indicating Don’t Know, 4 indicating Agree Just a Little, 5 indicating Agree, and 6 indicating Strongly Agree. Parents were asked to rate each statement based on how much they disagree or agree. The survey takes approximately 30 minutes to complete.

The Parental Involvement Project (PIP) questionnaire had established reliability and validity (Hoover-Dempsey, Sandler, & Walker, 2005) and was used in this study to assess key variables posed in the TPB model (i.e., parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and ascertain whether they were significantly related to and could predict the reported intentions of involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in their children’s early childhood education programs. The survey had five sections: (1) parental attitudes and beliefs, (2) subjective norms, (3) perceived behavioral controls, (4) parent’s intention to become involved, and (5) household demographics information.

The construct of parental attitudes and beliefs is a learned predisposition to respond to an object or class of objects in a consistently favorable or unfavorable way (Kiriakidis, 2015). This variable was measured by using statements that reflected parental attitudes and beliefs. The goal was to ascertain individual’s behavioral attitudes and beliefs toward getting involved in children’s education and the patterns of parental behavior that followed the beliefs based on parents’ motivations for being involved (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005). This subscale included 24 items (e.g., My child’s learning is mainly up to the teacher and my child) with responses ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The subscale score for these items ranges from 24 to 144.

The construct of subjective norms is an individual’s perception of significant others’ beliefs; how much a person is influenced by the judgment of significant others such as teachers, parents, friends, or spouse (Ajzen, 1991). The subscale consisted of six statements constructs to reflect subjective norms (e.g., I think most parents at my child’s center are actively involved) with responses ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The subscale score for these items ranges from 6 to 36.

The construct of perceived behavioral controls (PBC) represents an individual’s perceived ease or difficulty of performing a particular behavior (Ajzen, 1991). Any behavior is rarely under complete volitional control and PBC can only be identified in relation to the individual. Many external and internal factors could potentially inhibit the intended execution of any behavior; therefore, the predictive role of PBC would depend on the degree to which the behavior was under volitional control and the potential role of external and internal factors to interfere with the behavior (Kiriakidis, 2015). Thus, the greater the behavior depended on these factors being enacted, the greater the predictive and explanatory role of PBC would be (Ajzen, 1991). These factors are assumed to reflect experience as well as anticipated impediments and obstacles. The subscale included 17 statements regarding parents’ ability to be involved (e.g., I have enough time and energy to attend special events at school) with responses ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The subscale score for these items ranges from 17 to 102.

The dependent variable of parental intentions represents an indication of an individual parent’s readiness to perform a given behavior, in this case parent involvement in schooling (Ajzen, 1991). Parental intentions are determined by attitudes and beliefs towards a parental behavior, subjective norms or pressures, and perceived control to perform the behavior and the parents’ motivation to comply (Kiriakidis, 2015). The subscale includes ten statements (e.g., Your child’s teacher asks you to schedule a conference to discuss your child’s progress) with responses ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The subscale score for these items ranges from 10 to 60.

The final section of the survey included household demographic questions requesting participants to report their family’s general information, such as their age, language spoken at home, and confirm their immigration/refugee status. Demographic data was collected to ensure all participants were at least 18 years and up and met criteria to be eligible for the study. The survey questions consistently reflected the construct that was measured (Field, 2013). The subscales have high internal consistency, the reliability of the proportion of variance attributable to the true score of the latent variable (Field, 2013). The reliability scale utilized was the Cronbach alpha (α) because the survey consisted of many Likert items (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). The coefficient alpha distinguished between the amount of variation, which stemmed from the latent variable, and the amount attributable to error. The alpha coefficient ranges in value from 0.0 to 1.0; however, when assessing the internal consistency, a scale bellowed .60 is unacceptable; between .60 and .65 is undesirable; between .65 and .70 is minimally acceptable; between .70 and .80 is respectable; and between .80 and .90 is magnificent (Field, 2013). The reliability of the scale was reported by Hoover-Dempsey, Sandler, and Walker (2002). The subscale reliability of the 24 items measuring attitudes and beliefs was acceptable (α = .77). The subjective norms subscale had a reported subscale reliability that was respectable (α = .78). The subscale of perceived behavioral control consists of 17 items and had a subscale reliability reported as α = .83. Finally, the parental intention subscale was composed of ten items with a reported subscale reliablility of α = .78 (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005).

Field (2013) stated that an instrument was valid when it measures what it set out to measure. For this study, the concern was whether the survey accurately measured parental involvement constructs of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and intentions. Empirical work on developing the constructs was included in the reports of Hoover-Dempsey and her colleagues (1995; 1997; 2002; 2005). They focused on the three factors of parents’ motivations and considered them the most useful in the model of parental involvement: (1) an active role construction of involvement (i.e., parents believe that being involved in their children’s education wass important to their learning development); (2) parental perceptions on being invited to be involved through teachers, school or office staff creating a social norm to encourage parent involvement; and (3) parents’ life context as critical. Parents’ understanding of their own skills and knowledge influenced their thinking about the types of involvement activities they took on. The parents’ perceptions on their available time and energy for involvement also influenced their decisions. The family culture played a significant role in their ideas about the ways they could be and were involved in supporting their child’s learning (e.g., even when children’s centers invite parents, their culture traditionally limited parent or caregivers’ role in children’s formal schooling) (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005). The design of this study included three independent variables and one dependent variable which are operationally defined in the following section.

 

 

Operational Definitions of Variables

The investigation included three independent variables considered as predictor variables and a single dependent variable as an outcome variable. The three independent variables were (a) attitudes and beliefs, (b) subjective norms, and (c) perceived behavior control. The single dependent variable was the parent’s reported intentions for involvement. Each variable was operationally defined as follows:

Attitudes/Beliefs. Parental attitudes and beliefs were measured using 24 items with responses ranging from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The individual items responses were summed for all items on the subscale to create an interval level measured for this variable. The subscale score for these items ranged from 24 to 144 with a low score indicating somewhat negative attitudes and beliefs regarding parent involvement and a high score indicating relatively positive attitudes and beliefs regarding parent involvement.

Subjective norms. Subjective norms were measured using six items constructed to reflect subjective norms. These responses ranged from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The individual item responses were summed for all items on the subscale to create an interval level measured for this variable. The subscale score for this variable ranged from 6 to 36 with a low score indicating somewhat negative subjective norms regarding parent involvement and a high score indicating relatively positive subjective norms regarding parent involvement.

Perceived behavioral controls. Perceived behavioral controls were measured using 17 items regarding the parents’ ability to get involved. The responses ranged from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The individual item responses were summed for all items on the subscale to create an interval level measure for this variable. The subscale score for these items ranged from 17 to 102 with a low score indicating little perceived control regarding parent involvement and a high score indicating considerable perceived control regarding parent involvement.

Parental intentions. Parental intentions was measured using ten items. The responses ranged from 1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree. The individual item responses were summed for all items on the subscale to create an interval level measured for this variable. The subscale score for these items ranged from 10 to 60 with a low score indicating weak intentions to engage in parent involvement and a high score indicating strong intentions to engage in parent involvement.

Study Procedures

The Northcentral University Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the research. The AKA Head Start executive director and all center directors also granted access and approval to conduct the study at their sites. The research materials and instruments were sent to all gatekeepers at the same time within an email. Each contained (a) a letter of asking permission to conduct the surveys (Appendix E), (b) a cover letter of an introduction to the research project and information about the nature and purpose of the study (Appendix D), and (c) a copy of the survey (Appendix A). The researcher met with the Head Start executive director and each of the 12 center directors, as first and second gatekeepers, to discuss whether the research inquiry could proceed and the process of how to contact parents/caregivers and distributed the surveys they would prefer. The final plan agreed upon for times of soliciting the participants was during the drop-off and pick-up times at each location. Once all approvals were received, the distribution of parent surveys began with a pre-notice letter (Appendix F). This pre-notice letter was put into each child’s mailbox at all 12 Head Start/Early Head Start centers for parents/caregivers. A few days after passing out the pre-notice letters, during the drop-off time at each of twelve centers, the researcher met parents or caregivers and briefly explained the purpose of the study and allowed them to ask any questions and asked if they would be interested in cooperating with the research voluntarily (Fowler, 2009). Those interested were given a survey package with (1) a survey, (2) a consent form (Appendix B), (3) a recruitment letter (Appendix G), and (4) a self-stamped envelope for participants to return the survey and consent signature to the researcher’s address if they prefer this mode. All materials were translated into their home language (i.e., Arabic, Spanish, and Vietnamese) to ensure parents/caregivers comprehended explanations thoroughly.

The recruitment letter communicated (a) the purpose of this study; (b) a request for their voluntary participation with no repercussions for not participating; (c) the importance and usefulness of participation, and (d) requirement of an informed consent document; (e) information of the risk level of involvement and absolute no-deception; (f) respondents’ names and information remaining confidential (i.e., names of respondents are not associated with the results in any reporting) and anonymous (i.e., names of respondents are not known); (g) the name and contact information of the research supervisor for this project at the Northcentral University (NCU) for further questions regarding the survey that respondents could use to inquire; and (h) a timeline for completing and returning the survey with an offered incentive for the survey respondents (Fowler, 2009). The researcher offered a gift card if the survey was completed on the same day or the participants took the surveys home and returned them within two to three weeks.

The next contact was a reminder postcard placed in each child’s mailbox that encouraged the participants who had not completed and returned the survey to do so if they were still interested in participating (Blaxter, Hughes, & Tight, 2014). The final contact was a thank-you letter for respondents’ time and consideration, accompanied with a gift card of ten dollars for those who returned the survey materials within the timeline. A gift card and a thank-you letter were sent to their home address by post office.

Data Collection and Analysis

All surveys were completed and collected within an eight-week timeframe. The survey results were entered into a spreadsheet. The data from the spreadsheet were entered into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 22 for final statistical analysis. A power analysis yielded an estimated sample size of 110 for a linear regression with three predictors (power of 0.8, type I error of 0.05, and medium size effect of 0.1) (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009); however, the researcher would had a goal of collecting data from at least 500 participants to account for missing data or incomplete surveys. The goal was to achieve a response rates between 30% and 40% (Fowler, 2009). A missing data rate of 15% to 20% is common in educational studies (Dong & Peng, 2013). Dong and Peng (2013) found that 36% of studies had no missing data, 48% had missing data, and about 16% could not be determined. Missing data reduces the statistical power of a trial and impacts to the quality of statistical inferences, which refers to the probability that would reject the null hypothesis (Dong & Peng, 2013; Kang, 2013). The missing rate of 5% or less was inconsequential; however, if there was more than this rate, then the bias of statistical analysis could likely happen (Dong & Peng, 2013). Moreover, the impact of missing data on quantitative research could be serious because there is loss of information, decreased statistical power, increased standard errors, and weakened generalizability of findings (Kang, 2013). During the survey process, missing data can be caused by several factors: (1) respondents refused or forgot to answer a question because of privacy issues, (2) the person taking the survey did not understand the question due to a lack of experience or reading skill, (3) respondents lost interest or did not have enough time to complete the questionnaire, (4) respondents did not show up on the survey day, and (5) databases had missing data because there was a mismatch of variables between databases.

The data was screened for accuracy to clarify any problems or errors (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). Missing data was handled by applying the multiple imputation approach (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). In this approach, replacing with a set of plausible values was the strategy for the missing values in which they contain natural variability and uncertainty of right values (Kang, 2013). The first step before a data set with missing values is analyzed by statistical procedures is that it needs to be edited in some ways into a complete data set. Failure to edit the data properly could make it unsuitable for a statistical procedure and the statistical analyses were vulnerable to violations of assumptions (Dong & Peng, 2013). Second, a prediction of the missing data was completed by using the existing data. Then the missing values were replaced with the predicted values, and a full data set called the imputed data set was created. This process iterates the repeatability and creates multiple imputed data sets (Kang, 2013). Kang stressed each multiple imputed data set produced be analyzed using the standard statistical analysis procedures for complete data, thereby giving multiple analysis results. By combining these analysis results, a single overall analysis result was produced. In addition to restoring the natural variability of the missing values, it incorporated the uncertainty due to the missing data, which results in a valid statistical inference (Kang, 2013). Moreover, restoring the natural variability of the missing data could be achieved by replacing the missing data with the imputed values using the regression method, or the predictive mean matching method ccould be used if the missing variables were continuous (Dong & Peng, 2013). Furthermore, multiple imputations are robust to the violation of the normality assumption and produces appropriate results even in the presence of a small sample size or a high number of missing data (Dong & Peng, 2013). SPSS has a missing value analysis module that allowed examination of the patterns of data completion in a descriptive way. This test identified whether the data significantly departed from missing data completely at random (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008). In general, the mean, standard deviation, and frequencies were checked on the amount of missing data, and the regression method was utilized to impute the missing values. Handling the missing data by using the SPSS Missing Value Analysis was helpful in solving any missing data problems (Kang, 2013). Cronbach’s alphas were used to compute and determine the internal consistency reliability of each subscale (Trochim & Donnelly, 2008).

Descriptive summary statistics, including the calculations of means, standard deviations, and ranges for attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavior control, and parental intentions were obtained to answer the first three research questions. Because this study had one dependent variable and multiple independent variables, a multiple regression analysis was used to determine the amount of variation in the dependent variable explained by the independent variables to answer research question four. The step-wise multiple regression employed had the following assumptions that must be met: linearity, collinearity, independence of errors, normality, and homoscedasticity. Statistical SPSS software was used to test each assumption. These assumptions were tested through a visual inspection of data plots, skewness, kurtosis, Q-plots, P-plots, and VIF statistics (Field, 2013). Skewness and kurtosis were checked in the statistical tables and normality was checked through histograms and plots of the standardized residuals (Field, 2013). If the assumption of normality was not possible reasons why were investigated (e.g., the underlying distribution was nonnormal, outliers or mixed distribution of scores on each of the variables might contain, a low discrimination gauge was used, skewness was present in the data) (Foster, Barkus, & Yavorsky, 2006) and then a corrective procedure was performed, such as transformations if needed. If the assumption of linearity was not met, then the item correlation matrix was examined to identify any item that did not correlated with the subscale and eliminate such items was considered with subscale reliabilities being recomputed (Foster, Barkus, & Yavorsky, 2006). If the assumption of no multicollinearity was not met, then the variables with a low value on tolerance would be removed. If the assumption of homoscedasticity was not met, then a transformation of the variables or use of a weighed least squares regression was considered (Foster, Barkus, & Yavorsky, 2006).

Summary statistics of demographics regarding ages and genders were computed and reported to describe the sample. To answer research question four, the SPSS analysis resulted in three main tables: the Model Summary table that reported the R and R^2 and adjusted R^2 and standard error to indicate how well the data fit the model, the ANOVA table that reported the F-ratio and significance for the overall regression model, and the Coefficients table that identified the coefficients for each independent variable and if it was a significant variable in the regression model (Laerd Statistics, n.d.).

Assumptions

The assumptions in this study included: (a) the application of the theory of planned behavior model was appropriate to be utilized in predicting/explaining and gaining a deeper understanding of immigrant and refugee parents/caregivers’ intentions with regard to parental involvement, (b) participants would answer all survey questions truthfully and honestly, (c) the variables of parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intention regarding parent involvement were complex and would be fully measured by the survey, (d) the survey instrument used was valid and reliable, and (e) the sampling was unbiased. These assumptions had a potential effect on the characteristics of data, such as distribution trends, correlational trends, and variable included.

Limitations

The following limitations were present in this study. A possible limitation for the immigrant and refugee parent participants was a language barrier. To compensate for this limitation, the survey documents were provided in English and in their primary language. Also, since the surveys were sent home, participants had no opportunity to clarify their confusion, if there was any, thereby possibly inaccurately interpreting one or more questions. Also, parental reading levels could impact comprehension of each question or prevent parents/caregivers from volunteering to take the survey (Keys, 2015). The researcher’s email address and phone numbers were provided for participants in case they had inquiries that arose regarding the survey during the study. Because the scope of this study included only a sample of immigrant/refugee families, the findings are limited to the sample obtained from those enrolled in Head Start and Early Head Start programs in the vicinity. This study was also limited by the use of a convenience sample. These limitations increased the possibility of common-method bias, which increased the probability that the characteristics of those who responded were different from those who did not. Alternatively, anonymity provided through anonymous survey helped counteract some biases vesus a focus groups of immigrants and refugees that would be susceptible to cultural correctness.

Delimitations

            The problem of parental involvement in children’s education in Head Start or Early Head Start programs was the focus of the study. Although there are other problems within immigrant/refugee families, the findings were delimited to only those variables being measured in relation to parent involvement and the theory of planned behavior model. The criteria for participants’ enrollment in this study were first-generation, foreign-born immigrant and refugee parents, which eliminated some parents who were not qualified to participate even though they would consider themselves a part of this population. This study was delimited to those who lived in a geographic regions in Southern California and whose children were enrolled in Head Start and Early Head Start programs.

            Regarding parental behavior, Ajzen’s theory of planned behavior (TPB) model (1991) was a useful method to explain and predict parents’ intentional behavior based on their personal beliefs about the outcomes of behaviors. The TPB was applied to describe the dynamic and complex nature of parental engagement in their child’s life and education (Bracke & Corts, 2012). In addition, the TPB based moldel offered a viable theoretical lens for examining parental involvement, the most important determinant of parental behavioral dispositions. However, based on these goals, the theory of planned behavior model may not captured all aspects of parents/caregivers and their intentions for parent involvement, yet the study delimited to only the three independent variables included in the model. These delimitations had a potential effect on the examination of relationships among beliefs/attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral controls, and parental intentions. Other variables may contribute to parent involvement or a lack of involvement that were not measured or were not accounted for in the study; therefore, the prediction formula is limited.

Ethical Assurances

            Given the study included human participants, ethical assurances were required. There were minimal elements of risk, and no deception was used in this study. Other assurances included privacy, informed consent, anonymity, secrecy, being truthful, and confidentiality (Moreno, Goniu, Moreno, & Diekema, 2013). Federal regulations defined a human subject as a living individual about whom an investigator obtained data through interaction with the individual or identifiable private information (Moreno, Goniu, Moreno, & Diekema, 2013). Informed consent, confidentiality, and protection of individuals were central to the guidelines on research ethics and were employed in the study (Blaxter, Hughes, & Tight, 2014). There was a statement in the recruitment letter for consent to participate; and the researcher required a signed informed consent form before collecting data from parents/caregivers and the consent forms and information were kept confidential and separate from data after collection (Moreno, Goniu, Moreno, & Diekema, 2013). The researcher’s assurances and trust included the absence of deception, the voluntary nature of participation, and the risk involved.

Further, it was critical to protect the participant identities (Fowler, 2009). Their names, email addresses, postal address and telephone numbers did not appear on the survey. Data and results from the survey did not include personal information, and the surveys were not shared with anyone other than the researcher and the committee at Northcentral University. Concerning participants’ information on all documents such as a consent form and demographic information, they will be destroyed after seven years of being kept in the researcher’s personal locked files (Fowler, 2009). The collected information was utilized only in support of this study and only for educational purposes. The final reports of the study contain no personal information of parents’/caregivers, maintaining their confidentiality.

Summary

To apply the theory of planned behavior model to predict/explain and gain a deeper understanding of the immigrant and refugee parents/caregivers’ intentions and behaviors with regard to parental involvement, it was necessary to utilize a quantitative method and a cross-sectional survey design. A descriptive and correlational design was utilized to identify the parents’ intention and behaviors and to answer the research questions. Upon receiving immigrant and refugee parents’ completed surveys, their responses were inputted into SPSS. The instrument used to collect data from parents or caregivers had four sections measuring perceptions of parents or caregivers: parental attitudes/beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral controls, and parental intentions. The instrument had sufficient reliability and validity. Descriptive statistics and inferential data analysis were used within SPSS to answer the research questions. The analyses included the calculations of means, standard deviations, frequencies, and percentages of parental intentions of involvement. Subscale reliabilities and correlations were calculated. A stepwise multiple regression was used to answer the final research question.

No comments:

Post a Comment