Tuesday, March 17, 2020


Early Childhood Education (2019) 1-19
_____________________________________________________________________________________________
Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Parental Involvement of Immigrant and Refugee Populations in Head Start
Anthony Tran
Northcentral University, Phoenix, AZ, USA
____________________________________________________________________________


Abstract


The purpose of this quantitative, correlational study was to assess the key variables of the theory of planned behavior as possible determinants of parental intentions for school involvement behavior specially for immigrant and refugee parents with children enrolled in Early Head Start and Head Start programs, and ascertain whether they are significantly correlated to and can predict reported intentions of parent involvement in their children’s school. Little is known about the theory of planned behavior’s ability to explain variance in the intentions of parental involvement toward children’s education. The variables in the TPB that contribute to parental intentions have not been thoroughly examined in the context of the target population. A sample of 122 parents completed a written questionnaire in four languages. The reported attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, behavioral control perceptions or intentions were significantly correlated with one another and contributed to the prediction of parental involvement by immigrant/refugee parents. The null hypothesis was rejected. The findings are particularly encouraging in that parents reported moderately high parental intentions, which will likely have a positive effect on their children’s education. Additionally, there is room for teachers and center directors to implement strategies that may lead parents to increase their level of involvement in a program. Future research should focus on: (1) further examination of demographic characteristics, (2) expanding the diversity and size of the sample for additional generalizability, (3) comparing immigrant and non-immigrant families regarding parents’ educational involvement practices in Head Start programs, and (4) improved measurements given the Parent Involvement Project (PIP) and its data collection protocol were not designed specifically with the TPB based model.
Key Words:
Attitude toward behavior; Behavioral belief; Behavior intention; Head Start; Early Head Start; Immigrants; Intention; Perceived behavioral control; Refugee; Southwest Asian groups; Subjective norm; Southeast Asian groups.
____________________________________________________________________________




Introduction
The Head Start and Early Head Start programs were designed for students to become ready upon enrolling in school (DeLoatche, Bradley-Klug, Ogg, Kromrey, & Sundman-Wheat, 2015; Dove, Neuharth-Pritchett, Wright, & Wallinga, 2015). However, while these programs showed generally positive results (DeLoatche et al., 2015), without parental involvement, these students were less ready to enter schooling at their elementary school age (Dove et al., 2015; Manz et al., 2014). Parental involvement is an important factor in helping to create successful early childhood education experiences (Garbacz et al., 2016; McCormick, Cappella, O'Connor, & McClowry, 2013; Smith, 2014). If parents are involved in their children’s preschool, then it bodes well for future involvement, academic success, and other positive outcomes for their children (Demircan & Erden, 2015). Nevertheless, immigrant and refugee families are of a particular concern in Head Start and Early Head Start programs because getting these parents involved is a challenge for many reasons (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013; Demircan & Erden, 2015). Barriers to parental involvement included parents’ pre-concieved beliefs about involvement, their current life circumstances, and their cultural beliefs (Manz, Gernhart, Bracaliello, Pressimone, & Eisenberg, 2014). Researchers have found a significant positive impact of parental involvement for toddlers at the beginning of early schooling (McCormick, Cappella, O’Connor, & McClowry, 2013). However, identifying ways to increase parental involvement with immigrant and refugee families has been difficult (DeLoatche et al., 2015; Porumbu & Necsoi, 2013; Pratt, Lipscomb, & Schmitt, 2015).
Attitudes and beliefs about the roles of parental involvement in education for immigrant or refugee parents might include that education is a teacher's responsibilities or that parents do not make a positive difference in their children's education (Bracke & Corts, 2012; Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013). Parental involvement in overall education relates to the beliefs of parents and whether they should or should not be involved in their children's education (Bracke & Corts, 2012). This knowledge serves as a role construction and is often shaped by a parent's personal experiences with schooling and personal perceptions (Bracke & Corts, 2012; Manz et al., 2014). If parents believe good parenting means that they should take an active role in their children's education, there is a greater likelihood they will play a decisive role in their children's education (Bracke & Corts, 2012). In the context of subjective norms about the roles of parents in education, parents may not be involved because they lack the examples of involvement or they come from a culture in which parents were never expected to be involved (Bracke & Corts, 2012). These norms are a product of whether an individual believes other meaningful people approve or disapprove of a behavior and their motivation to comply with those people (Perry & Langley, 2013). Parental intentions are a direct function of the attitude about the behavior and represent an indication of a parent’s readiness to perform a given behavior, such as how the responding parents plans on being involved during the school year (Bracke & Corts, 2012; Kiriakidis, 2015). Finally, parents’ perceptions of their personal level of control over their choices for involvement and the barriers to participation that they encounter contribute to their level of participation within the school (Ajzen, 1991). For example, low-income parents may have restrictive jobs that limit their availability, thereby giving an obstacle to their involvement (Bracke & Corts, 2012). Considered holistically, researchers have suggested that parents or caregivers of immigrant or refugee children would need to be involved in their children’s education to promote success; however, few studies exist specifically assessing and addressing immigrant and refugee populations who are at risk of low parental involvement with regard to education of toddlers and preschoolers (Manz et al., 2014).
Parental involvement in schooling activities for toddlers and preschoolers is important to a child’s outcomes (e.g., higher levels of proactive learningbehaviors,greater receptive vocabulary, and lower levels of conduct problems) (Bracke & Corts, 2012; Hindman et al., 2012; Manz et al., 2014). Particularly, parents who are immigrants or refugees, for example someone who was foreign born (first-generation immigrant) (Krogstad, 2015; Winsler et al., 2014), often have low involvement in their children’s education, possibly due to their attitudes and beliefs (Demircan & Erden, 2015; Manz et al., 2014; Poureslami et al., 2013). Use of a relevant theory may guide the design of an intervention to improve parental participation for a particular population (Lin, 2012). The theory of planned behavior (TPB) might be useful to examine parental behaviors, but this theory has been applied in a limited way to early childhood education settings (McGregor & Knoll, 2015). There was a need for research concerning parental involvement with the All Kids Academy (AKA) Early Head Start and Head Start programs, particularly within the immigrant and refugee communities.
Significance of the Study
Parental involvement has been recognized as a variable of significant impact on early childhood education (McCormick, Cappella, O’Connor, & McClowry, 2013). It has been found to increase the academic functioning of young children, especially for immigrant children who grow up with at least one foreign-born parent and speak a home language other than English (Demircan & Erden, 2015; Winsler et al., 2014). Therefore, it was important to study the involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in preschool because their historical, socio-demographic characteristics and academic concerns are possibly different with regards to parental attitudes and beliefs, perceived control, subjective norms, and subsequent behavioral intentions (Hindman et al., 2012). A lack of parental involvement was a factor in at-risk children’s academic achievement (Demircan & Erden, 2015; Hindman et al., 2012). Parent/caregiver involvement has improved children’s success (Bracke & Corts, 2012; Demircan & Erden, 2015; Porumbu & Necşoi, 2013). Little is known about how the theory of planned behavior might explain parent involvement of immigrant and refugee children at Early Head Start and Head Start programs (Ntuli, Nyarambi, & Traore, 2014). This quantitative study contributed to understanding parents’ perceptions of their involvement in Head Start programs and factors that might contribute to their involvement. The research was important because it builds greater understanding of how or if parental involvement and engagement practices for immigrant/refugee parents or caregivers could be effective. The findings could be used to encourage school and childcare center directors and teachers to expand their repertoire of strategies to involve parents from diverse language, cultural, and ethnic backgrounds. Identifying the determinants of parent involvement, or lack thereof, in immigrant and refugee populations that could subsequently be used to develop interventions was an important goal of the study (Hindman et al., 2012). Implications for improving current parent involvement policies and practices for engaging immigrants and refugees were developed from the findings and could be useful in overcoming the barriers to involvement if the schools wish to improve their programs. Subsequently, these parents might be able to better support their children and the school (Bulotsky-Shearer et al., 2012; Smith, 2014).
The testing of Ajzen’s TPB with regard to parent involvement in early childhood programs has never been tested relative to immigrant and refugee parents/caregivers in the context of Head Start and Early Head Start programs; therefore, more information concerning parents’ attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral controls, and subsequent behavioral intentions can contribute to improving the effectiveness of the program. By using Ajzen’s TPB model (1991), the results from this study contributed to the knowledge of potential outcomes of parental involvement, such as the following: affirmation of parents’ and caregivers’ positive attitudes about school participation, clarification of the dissonance between parents’ professed attitudes, beliefs, values, and intentions and in their actual behaviors, and rationale for a norm-based initiative that might increase parental involvement (Bracke & Corts, 2012).
Statement of the Problem
The problem to be addressed in this study was the limitation of immigrant and refugee parent involvement in Early Head Start and Head Start to support their child’s education (Gennetian et al., 2019; Hindman et al., 2012; Jeon et al., 2018; Manz et al., 2014). Specifically, this study addressed a lack of knowledge regarding what variables were related to high and/or low parent involvement and identification of those that might serve as barriers to or promoters of parent involvement in this population. The lack of knowledge mostly surrounded which, if any, variables were related to parents’ beliefs about whether they “should” or “should not” be involved in their children’s education were specifically among immigrant and refugee parents who might or might not engage in parent involvement activities differently than other parents given their different personal experiences and perceptions about schooling (Brace & Corte, 2012). Some of the barriers for immigrant and refugee families might be related to their pre-conceived beliefs about education (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013) as well as differences due to language and culture (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013). The variables as defined within the theory of planned behavior (TPB), that contribute to parental intentions are attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control (Ajzen, 1991; Bracke & Corts, 2012; Perry & Langley, 2013), and these have not been thoroughly examined within this parent population. Such knowledge could hopefully be applied to help program educators devise interventions for parents (McGregor & Knoll, 2015) to improve their intention for involvement in schooling. Without a further understanding of how these variables are related to and predict parental intentions for involvement, strategies employed by these programs to increase the parental involvement of immigrant and refugee families might be less effective and the families might not take full advantage of these programs (Lee & Zhou, 2014).
Purpose of the Study

The purpose of this quantitative, correlational study was to assess the key variables of the theory of planned behavior as possible determinants of parental intentions for school involvement behavior (i.e., parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and ascertain whether they were significantly related to and could predict the reported intentions of involvement of immigrant and refugee parents in their children’s early childhood education programs. By assessing the constructs that are pivotal to TPB, a test of this theory within the context of immigrant and refugee families with children enrolled in AKA Head Start or Early Head Start can be accomplished. Using a survey instrument to collect data, the goals were to (1) identify parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, parental intention, and perceived behavioral control regarding parent involvement, and (2) investigate how, if at all, these variables are correlated with and served to predict parent involvement, as TPB would suggest, in this population. The dependent variable was the level of intention for parental involvement, and the predictors were the determinants of behaviors as outlined in TPB (i.e., parent attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control) and reported by immigrant and refugee families with children enrolled in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs.

The population under study targeted the approximately 800-1000 parents/caregivers who were foreign born, i.e., first-generation immigrants or refugees (Krogstad, 2015; Winsler et al., 2014), living in a region of Southern California, and whose children were enrolled in either an Early Head Start or Head Start program. A census of this population was conducted with the goal of obtaining a sample of 122 parents/caregivers whose children participated in these programs. A power analysis using G-Power software yielded an estimated sample size of 110 for a linear regression with three predictors (power of 0.8, type one error of 0.05, and medium size effect) (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009). Parents were asked to complete the Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire, a 57-item survey that has been found to be reliable and valid for measuring attitudes and beliefs (24 items), subjective norms (6 items), perceived behavioral control (17 items), and parental intentions (10 items) for parent involvement. All items were used a six-point Likert scales. Data collection provided an opportunity to assess how these variables presented in immigrant and refugee families and examine their ability to predict parent intentions towards involvement. Step-wise multiple regression was used to assess the significance of the contributions of each predictor to explain the variation in the dependent variable.
 
Conceptual Framework
Ajzen's theory of planned behavior (TPB) (1991) was the framework employed to explain and predict intentional behavior based on personal beliefs about outcomes of behaviors. His theory was developed in the late 1980s as an extension of the theory of reasoned action. Ajzen described the dynamic and complex nature of parental engagement in a child’s life and education (Bracke & Corts, 2012). The TPB offered a viable theoretical lens for examining parental involvement and identifying the determinants among the immigrant and refugee parent population. To understand planned behavior and parental involvement, social attitudes and personality traits were included in an attempt to predict and explain human behavior (Ajzen, 1991). The TPB focused on the motivations of an individual to engage in a particular behavior, and the theory had the potential to be a useful framework for conducting research into parental involvement (Perry & Langley, 2013).
The central tenet of the theory is the intention to perform a particular behavior as defined by three independent determinants of intentions: attitude and belief toward the behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control (Ajzen, 1991). First, attitude referred to an individual’s values for the outcomes of behavior as associated with his or her beliefs related to the behavior and his or her evaluation of performing a behavior. Second, subjective norms were an individual’s perceptions of social pressures that existed for performing the behavior. This concept was comprised of beliefs about social expectations and the need to adhere to those expectations. Third, perceived behavioral control was related to an individual’s perception of how difficult the task would be to perform (Ajzen, 1991). Furthermore, the theory was based on beliefs about factors that were for or against performing the behavior and the perceived power of those factors. In general, the intention of performing a behavior was strong when the performance of a particular behavior elicited a favorable attitude from the individual was positively associated with subjective norms, and it was easy to perform with few obstacles (Ajzen, 1991). If the surrounding social environment was conducive to the behavior, and the individual was confident in their ability to perform the behavior then the person's intention was stronger to engage in the behavior, and thus, it was more likely he or she would (Ajzen, 1991).
Ajzen stressed that interventions designed to change behavior could be directed at one or more of its determinants (attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, or perceptions of behavioral control). Changes in one of these determinants should produce changes in behavioral intentions and, given adequate control over the behavior, intentions should be carried out under appropriate circumstances (Ajzen, 1991). Subsequent parental involvement might succeed in producing corresponding changes in attitudes, subjective norms, and perceptions of behavioral control because intentions were directed by behavioral, normative, and control beliefs (Kiriakidis, 2015). However, the behavioral intention might vary towards different behaviors and within different populations. For example, intention to perform one behavior might be primarily determined by the attitude toward the behavior, while another behavioral intention might be determined largely by normative influence. Similarly, intention to perform a particular behavior might be primarily under the attitudinal influence in one population, while more influenced by normative influence in another population (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2015). Thus, to design effective interventions to influence behavioral intentions, it is important first to determine the degree to which intention is influenced by attitudes and beliefs, perceived norms, and perceived behavioral controls (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2015). Parent interventions would be ineffective unless individuals were in fact capable of carrying out their newly formed intentions (Girardelli & Patel, 2016). Ajzen (1991) also noted that there should be an active link from intentions to behavior to facilitate the success of an intervention (Ajzen, 1991). One of the most efficient ways to intervene to help develop intentions is to induce individuals to form a plan with specific details of when, where, and how the desired behavior would be performed (Lin, 2012).
Girardelli and Patel (2016) provided the importance of behavioral intentions in the function of the three constructs of attitudes toward the target behavior, perceived norms, and perceived behavioral control/self-efficacy. Attitudes toward the target behavior were defined as a latent disposition or tendency to respond with some degree of favorableness or unfavorableness to a psychological object. Perceived norms referred to what was considered an acceptable or permissible behavior in a group or society. Perceived norms captured the total social pressure that the environment exerts on an individual to perform, or not perform a given behavior. Perceived behavioral control is defined as the extent to which people believed they are capable of performing a given behavior, that they have control over its performance.
Conducting research on parental involvement, using Ajzen’s theory, in immigrant and refugee families might be useful (Perry, 2013) because through this perspective, benefits for the child, family, and school could be encouraged (Bulotsky-Shearer et al., 2012). However, to apply the TPB based model to immigrant and refugee families such as African, Asian, or Hispanic/Latino, researchers need a better understanding of the issues in practice for these populations (Paat, 2013), since they might have several specific factors that impede family involvement such as a lack of knowledgeable resources and language, cultural, and physical barriers (Garbacz et al., 2016). From these points of view, the gaps often happen between this theory and its practices (Dobson & Beshai, 2013) because there are a number of issues, controversies, and assumptions involving the translation of theory into practice (Udo-Akang, 2012). However, the gap between theory and practice was framed as a knowledge transfer problem, and researcher’s continuous refinement and development based on learning from the application of theory into the field were never completed (Udo-Akang, 2012). Thus, it was important to choose theory testing rather than theory building because the theory was needed in explaining and predicting parental involvement with the TPB in distinct populations (Udo-Akang, 2012). It was critical to investigate and understand the behavior from the perspective of the study population because some individuals in certain demographic groups might hold beliefs about positive outcomes of the behavior and thus hold more positive attitudes and stronger intention to carry out the behavior. In addition, they might be more likely than others to engage in the behavior (Montano & Kasprzyk, 2015).
Researchers have examined parental involvement through the application of other theories to include identifying theory, parental investment theory, socio-ecological theory, role theory and social exchange theory; however, the theories have been limited in explaining and predicting parental involvement in early childhood education (Perry & Langley, 2013). According to Perry and Langley (2013), such theories assumed that active involvement of parents only relied on the parents' will and desired to be involved in their children's education. As an alternative theory was more helpful to explain and predict parental involvement, Perry and Langley (2013) suggested using Ajzen's TPB model and stressed that this model was “versatile enough to account for the dynamic and complex nature of parental engagement” (p. 181). What remained unknown was whether the TPB model would hold for parental engagement in schools with parents of particular cultures and status, such as those with immigrant and refugee status.
Nature of the Study
In the current study, Ajzen’s version of TPB (1991) was used as a basis of a model to examine how intentional behaviors of parents and personal beliefs about outcomes of their behaviors might affect their perceived levels of participation in their child’s school. The model constructed for this study included the TPB constructs of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions for involvement (Bracke & Corts, 2012). The descriptive and correlational design allowed for assessing the key variables posed in the TPB model as determinants of behavior (i.e., parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control) and then analysis of whether they were significantly related to and could predict the intentions of immigrant and refugee parents to be involved in their children’s early childhood education programs. The dependent variable were the parent’s reported intentions for involvement, and the predictors would be the determinants of behaviors as outlined in TPB, which were parental attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control (Bracke & Corts, 2012).
The correlational design used in this study was appropriate for testing whether TPB constructs fit a model for an observed set of relationships among the constructs in the model (Girardelli & Patel, 2016). Survey methods were used to measure the four constructs of TPB with the three-predictor variables and the single dependent variable of parent intention. The survey method allowed objectivity and minimized bias (Smith, 2015). The quantitative design-controlled biases so that facts, instances, and phenomena could be understood in an objective way (Park & Park, 2016). This design best aligned with the purpose and goals of the study. After receiving approval from Northcentral University’s Institutional Review Board (IRB) and the AKA Head Start administrative office, parents were solicited at each of the centers. During the drop off time at each of 12 centers, the researcher briefly explained the purpose of the study to the parents/caregivers and asked them to voluntarily participate if they were interested in cooperating with the research (Fowler, 2009). The consent form informed them of the voluntary nature of participation, the level of involvement, the absence of deception, and that their participation would remain anonymous. When consent was granted, volunteers were asked to complete a brief demographic information questionnaire regarding their immigrant/refugee status. Eligible participants received a PIP survey package with a cover letter. The cover letter to parents detailed the purpose of the study. All papers were translated into their home language (i.e., Arabic, Spanish or Vietnamese) to ensure parents/caregivers understand explanations clearly. Self-addressed and stamped envelopes were enclosed for returning the survey.
Research Questions
Q1. What are the attitudes and beliefs of immigrant/refugee parents regarding parent involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start as determined by Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire?
Q2. What are the subjective norms of immigrant/refugee parents regarding parental involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start as determined by Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire?
Q3. What are the perceived behavioral control perceptions of immigrant/refugee parents regarding parent involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs as determined by Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire?
Q4. What are the parental intentions of immigrant/refugee parents regarding parent involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs as determined by Parental Involvement Project (PIP) Parent Questionnaire?
Q5. What is the relationship of measures of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavior control, and how well, if at all, do they predict parental intentions to parental involvement by parents in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs?
 
Hypotheses
H0. The attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, intentions, behavioral control perceptions, and perceived control of immigrant and refugee parents are not significantly related to (p > .05) or cannot predict parents’ level of involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs.
Ha. One or more of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, intentions, behavioral control perceptions, or perceived behavior control perceptions are significantly correlated to (p < .05) and significantly contribute to the prediction of parental involvement by immigrant/refugee parents in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs.
Methodology
This study used a quantitative non-experimental correlation. The method was used a self-administered paper-based survey to identify and access the sample. Parents returned a survey and received $10 gift card incentive.
Population
The population included all immigrant/refugee parents from AKA HS programs in southern California.
Sample
The sample was available to obtain in person at the 12 sites of AKA HS. The executive director and center directors were asked to collaborate with the project to ensure all immigrant/refugee parents or caregivers have a chance to participate. This way, avoided a bias that affects the relationship between a sample of respondents and the population.
A total of 500 parent surveys were sent out to parents who met the criteria in which parents to participate are first-generation and considered an immigrant or refugee and have been living in Southern California and whose children are enrolled in either an HS or EHS program.
There were 122 parents successfully completed the surveys by answering all the items. It was 24% survey’s packages returned. Including Arabic (12%), Hispanic (41%), Vietnamese (6%), and English speaking (41%). All of them were between 18 to 65 years old.
Instrument
The Parent Involvement Project (PIP) survey was the instrument used for data collection. The questionnaire contained 57 items developed by Hoover-Dempsey, Walker, Jones, and Reed (2002). All items were measured on a six-point Likert scale with subscales for attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and parental intentions for involvement.
The Likert scale ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree, with the additional option of I Don’t Know. For example, parents were asked to rate each statement based on how much they disagree or agree. The options were 1 indicating Strongly Disagree, 2 indicating Disagree, 3 indicating Don’t Know, 4 indicating Agree Just a Little, 5 indicating Agree, and 6 indicating Strongly Agree.
All surveys were collected within an eight-week timeframe. Once the survey’s time had passed, the survey results were entered into Statistical Package Social Science (SPSS), version 22 for statistical analysis. All analyses were conducted using the 0.05 level of significance.
Descriptive summary statistics, means, standard deviations, and ranges for attitudes/beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral controls, and parental intentions were obtained to answer the first four research questions.
A Pearson’s correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to determine the amount of variation in the dependent variable explained by the three independent variables.
Results
Attitudes & Beliefs
The result of research question one related to the attitudes/beliefs with the means of 107, standard deviation was14, and the range was 120. The mean score was considered moderate.
Subjective Norms
The result of research question two related to subjective norms with the means of 31, standard deviation was four, the range was 30. The mean score was considered moderately high.
Perceived Behavioral Controls
The results of research question three related to the perceived behavioral controls with the mean of 88, standard deviation was nine, and the range was 85. The mean score was considered moderate.
Parental intentions
The result of research question four related to the parental intentions with the means of 51, standard deviation was six, the range was 50. The mean score was considered moderately high.
Discussion
The attitudes/beliefs parents reported was moderate (M=107). This level implies that parents from immigrant/refugee populations hold generally positive attitudes about becoming involved in the early education of their children and believe that parent involvement is important to their children’s success in school.
The subjective norms parents reported somewhat high (M=31). This level implies that parental involvement does not seem to be an area in which parents from immigrant/refugee communities are concerned with regard to making sure they are following cultural or communities’ norms.
The perceived behavioral control parents reported was moderate (M=88). This level implies that these parents do believe they have some degree of control over their ability to become involved in their child’s education. This would influence parents’ thinking about the kinds of involvement activities they can take on or choose to engage in.
The parental intentions reported were moderately high (M=51). This implies that parents from immigrant/refugee populations appear to indicate they are moderately high interested in becoming involved in their child’s early education.
The null hypothesis for research question five was rejected with 50% of the variance in intentions accounted for by all three variables. It was rejected because statistically significant positive correlations were found among the variables.
 
Practical Implications
Immigrant and refugee parents’ involvement in HS/EHS programs can be predicted by their reported attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral controls with regard to their involvement in children’s education. This finding implies that all three variables should be considered important aspects by which teachers, center directors, and administrators may affect parents’ intentions and desire to be involved in their child’s schooling. The findings of the multiple regression suggested that attitudes and beliefs and subjective norms were the two strongest predictors of parental intentions for involvement. Some prior researchers also found all three variables to be significant predictors of parental intentions for involvement (Kiriakidis, 2015; Tipton, 2014), while others have had findings that support only one or two of these variables (Case, Spark, & Pavey, 2016). The findings supported that the theory of planned behavior can be applied to explaining parent involvement of immigrants and refugees whose children are enrolled in HS/EHS, thereby expanding the theory to encompass people with culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds.
This study was guided by the theory of planned behavior that stipulates the more favorable parents’ intention to engage in their child’s education, based on their attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control, the more likely they will actually engage with their intended behaviors (Ajzen, 2011). The findings of this study revealed the implications of improving current parent involvement policies for engaging immigrants and refugees, and they may be able to overcome the barriers of involvement if the schools can improve their programs. Regarding parental behavior, Ajzen’s theory of planned behavior (TPB) model (1991) was a useful method to explain and predict parents’ intentional behavior based on their personal beliefs about the outcomes of behaviors. It was used to describe the dynamic and complex nature of parental engagement in their child’s life and education (Bracke & Corts, 2012). In addition, the TPB based model offers a viable theoretical lens for examining parental involvement, the most important determinant of parental behavioral dispositions. The findings from this study are also particularly encouraging in that they reported to have relatively moderate parental intentions, and this will likely have a positive effect on their child’s education if teachers and center directors lead parents to increase their level of involvement in a program.
Recommendations for Practice
By assessing the constructs that were pivotal to TPB, an expansion of this theory––within the context of immigrant and refugee families with children enrolled in Head Start or Early Head Start—was accomplished. There was a lack of proper application of TPB highlighted in the literature review about both of school personnel and key variables in this study (Dean, Stewart, & Debattista, 2017). The aspects of parental involvement included attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and subsequent parental intentions. First, parents or caregivers need the provision of resources to change their attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control toward their children’s education in early development. Second, increasing parent involvement is positively correlated with higher achievement of children’s learning activities (Castro et al., 2015). Thus, teachers and center directors must outweigh the challenges that they encounter with involving parents. Any barrier identified by parents should be resolved and educators should strive to make a priority of reaching out to involve parents (DeLoatche, Bradley-Klug, Ogg, Dromrey, & Sundman-Wheat, 2015).
      Researchers have reported on different methods of involving parents that play a positive role in becoming involved in their children’s school (Kikas et al., 2014). Castro et al. (2015) conducted a meta-analysis and found multiple ways in which families could be involved in children’s learning at home, in the community, and in school. At home, a family’s engagement is the most important factor to a child’s development; for example, the activities include shared book reading, parent-child conversation, discussed letters and sounds, and writing exercises. Hindman, Miller, Froyen, and Skibbe (2012) stressed that enjoyable at-home learning activities may encourage children’s positive attitudes about learning. In the community, families can help children learn about the wider world and access resources that may not be readily available within the household such as visiting libraries, attending museums, sport events, church functions, or other cultural opportunities (Hindman et al., 2012). School-based involvement includes various activities in which parents engage, for example, participating in school trips, volunteering in the classroom or at school events, fundraising, and attending school programs (Castro et al., 2015; Kikas et al., 2014; McCormick et al., 2013). In school-based activities, McCormick, Cappella, O’Connor, and McClowry (2013) found that parents who were involved would likely have positive relationships with teachers; in turn, teachers might be less liable to perceive problematic behaviors among the children of highly involved parents. Parents and caregivers could volunteer in the classroom or staff the office to participate in decision-making bodies such as the parent policy council or personal communication such as parent-teacher conferences (Hindman et al., 2012).
      Homeschool conferencing is a communication between parents and school staff on educational topics related to a specific child (Castro et al., 2015; Kikas et al., 2014). For school conferences or meetings, where oral communication skills are essential, parents with limited English language skills can be asked in advance to bring an adult whom they trust to serve as their translator (Cheatham & Ostrosky, 2013). If centers have trouble-locating translators for written school materials, schools should successfully collaborate with community-based organizations and refugee resettlement agencies to provide translation assistance (Manz et al., 2014). Finally, centers should outreach to families through informal meeting settings. For example, making home visits for young children below the age of three years is a primary means of strengthening the pivotal role of parents (DeLoatche et al., 2015; Manz et al., 2014). Although encouraging parent involvement was politically neutral and rhetorically popular, much of the research informing policy was occurring in the absence of clarity around the dimensions of parent involvement and the role of teachers in predictive relationships of children’s behaviors (McCormick et al., 2013). Researchers found that preschool educators and staff should be trained to communicate with parents or caregivers regarding giving their engagement in home-school learning activities, as well as in the communities (DeLoatche et al., 2015).
Positive attitudes and beliefs of parents and caregivers toward participation in Head Start and Early Head Start programs have benefited them. Parental involvement and acceptance of these family characteristics are important aspects of research and can always be improved for these actions ultimately can affect immigrant and refugee parents’ perspectives about their child’s education (Leyendecher et al., 2018). When dealing with immigrant and refugee families with different characteristics, Head Start faculties may not understand the difficulties the young children and families have been through (DeLoatche et al., 2015) and may not always able to address specific needs that families wants. Hence, knowledge of parent involvement is crucial to Head Start staff who should be well-trained (Edberg et al., 2017).
For parental involvement, attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control play a crucial role in parental intentions to be involved and, subsequently, the development of their children. Of all the variables that were considered important, parental involvement was considered to have the greatest impact on preschoolers’ education (Dean, Stewart, & Debattista, 2017). There is evidence that parents strongly believe they should be involved in helping their children succeed in school. Therefore, administrator or center directors should share this same regard for involvement and set the tone for a school culture that has high expectations for parental involvement. Collaboration will be needed between parents and teachers to maintain the expectation in the Head Start and Early Head Start centers. Teachers and center directors should consider the theory of planned behavior to account for parental intentions to become and remain involve with their child. Chiefly, teachers and center directors should communicate clearly that all parents have an important role to play in children’s learning success.
Recommendations for Future Research
      Continued research on parental involvement is needed to further support a significant correlation with and contribution to the prediction of parental involvement by immigrant/refugee parents in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs. The problem of parental involvement in children’s education in Head Start or Early Head Start programs was a primary focus in the study, although other problems arise in immigrant/refugee families. Findings were delimited to only those variables being measured in relation to parent involvement and the theory of planned behavior. The criteria for participants’ enrollment in this study were immigrants and refugees, which eliminated some families from participating even though they would consider themselves a part of this population. This study delimited the population to only the geographic regions in southern California being sampled and whose children were enrolled in Head Start and Early Head Start programs. Expansion to a wider geographic region, other programs that serve the early childhood population and more diverse parent populations would serve to further validate the current findings.
      The current study has only focused on the three factors contributing to parent intentions for involvement as defined by TPB, but other factors may affect parental intentions for involvement. This delimitation had a potential effect on the examination of relationships among beliefs/attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral controls, and parental intentions. One effect was that other variables could contribute to parent involvement or a lack of involvement that were not measured or not included in TPB and so were not accounted for in the study; therefore, the prediction formula would be limited. Future research should attempt to identify additional factors that might be particular to one people group. Researcher seeking to gain a better understanding of demographic characteristics that may conflict with parental involvement and parental intention outcomes would serve to further inform TPB (DeLoatche et al., 2015).
      Another recommendation would be for studies to examine the effect of biases and prejudices based on languages, races, religions, and socioeconomic statuses. Including a wider sample of a diverse demographic population, such as Chinese, Somalians, or Iranians for more findings would also be worthwhile. Investigations of both immigrant and non-immigrant families’ educational involvement practices in Head Start programs might also serve to reveal differences that may need to be addressed by program directors to further increase involvement (Sibley & Brabeck, 2017). More research is needed to explore teacher-parent relationships that are developed within the programs, particularly with the immigrant and refugee populations, and in which teachers’ and parents’ perceptions of the program. Research on the teacher-parent relationship could advance understanding of the benefit of having positive intentions related to parents’ involvement in the HS/EHS programs (DeLoatche et al., 2015; Perry & Langley, 2013).
Conclusions
This quantitative correlational study utilized a correlation analysis to measure the degree of an association among the variables and a multivariate regression analysis to assess the magnitude with which TPB variables could predict parental intentions for involvement in schooling. The measures of attitudes and beliefs, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control perceptions, and intentions of immigrant and refugee parents were significantly related to each other and predicted 50% of the variance in parents’ intentions for involvement in AKA Head Start and Early Head Start programs resulting in rejection of the null hypothesis. The findings are consistent with the theory of planned behavior applied to explain parent involvement for parents with culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds. The variables examined significantly predicted the reported intentions of parental involvement in early childhood education programs. The findings of this study would support the theory of planned behavior as applied to parent involvement of immigrant and refugee parents whose children are enrolled in HS/EHS. Information collected in the study is useful for training for Head Start educators in order to meet the needs of immigrant and refugee families and involve parents. Competence training should be provided in HS/EHS programs.
Future research on parental involvement in the HS/EHS programs should continue. The most effective efforts for improving parental involvement must be reinforced and built in early childhood education by a program executive director, a center director, educators, and policy-makers. These professionals always wish to increase parental involvement in the programs to be successful; however, the number of immigrants and refugees’ families is rapidly growing each year, which affects the way HS/EHS programs operate. Finding an appropriate method of increasing parental intentions to participate in their children’s education helps address the centers’ responsibility of increasing parent involvement and the immigrant and refugee family would benefit. An important task to promoting children’s early education is getting parents involved in their programs.


 

References

Bracke, D., & Corts, D. (2012). Parental involvement and the theory of planned behavior. Education, 133(1), 188-201. Retrieved from https://morganearnold.files.wordpress.com/2012/09/parental-involvment-and-the-theory-of-planned-behavior.pdf

Bulotsky-Shearer, R., Wen, X., Faria, A.-M., Hahs-Vaughn, D. L., & Korfmacher, J. (2012). National profiles of classroom quality and family involvement: A multilevel examination of proximal influences on Head Start children’s school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 27, 627-639. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.02.001

Case, P., Sparks, P., Pavey, L. (2016). Identity appropriateness and the structure of the theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(1), 109-125. doi:10.1111/bjso.12115

Castro, M., Exposito-Casas, E., Lopez-Martin, E., Lizasoain, L., Navarro-Asencio, E., & Gaviria, J. L, (2015). Parental involvement on student academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review 14, 33-46. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2015.01.002

Cheatham, G. A. & Ostrosky, M. M. (2013). Goal setting during early childhood parent-teacher conferences: A comparison of three groups of parents. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 27(2), 166-189. doi:10.1080/02568543.2013.767291

Connor P. (2016). International migration: Key findings from the U.S., Europe and the world. PewResearchCenter. Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/12/15/international-migration-key-findings-from-the-u-s-europe-and-the-world/

Dean, J., Mitchell, M., Stewart, D., & Debattista, J. (2017). Intergenerational variation in sexual health attitudes and beliefs among Sudanese refugee communities in Australia. Culture, health & sexuality19(1), 17-31. doi:10.1080/13691058.2016.1184316

DeLoatche, K. J., Bradley-Klug, K. L., Ogg, J., Kromrey, J. D., & Sundman-Wheat, A. N. (2015). Increasing parent involvement among Head Start families: A randomized control group study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43(4), 271-279. doi:10.1007/s10643-014-0660-7

Demircan, Ö., & Erden, F. T. (2015). Parental involvement and developmentally appropriate practices: A comparison of parent and teacher beliefs. Early Child Development and Care, 185(2), 209-225. doi:10.1080/03004430.2014.919493

Dove, M. K., Neuharth-Pritchett, S., Wright, D. W., & Wallinga, C. (2015). Parental involvement routines and former Head Start children’s literacy outcomes. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 29(2), 173-186. doi:10.1080/02568543.2015.1011360

Edberg, M. C., Cleary, S. D., Andrade, E. L., Evans, W. D., Simmons, L. K., & Cubilla-Batista, I. (2017). Applying ecological positive youth development theory to address co-occurring health disparities among immigrant Latino youth. Health promotion practice18(4), 488-496. doi:10.1177/1524839916633802

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G-Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 41(4), 1149-1160. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th ed.). SAGE. New Delhi, India.

Foster, J., Barkus, E., & Yavorsky, C. (2006). Understanding and using advanced statistics. SAGE Publications, Inc. CA: Thousand Oaks.

Fowler, F. J. (2009). Survey Research Methods. (4th ed.). SAGE Publications, Inc., CA: Thousand Oaks.

Garbacz, S. A., McIntosh, K., Eagle, J. W., Dowd-Eagle, S. E., Hirano, K. A., & Ruppert, T. (2016). Family engagement within schoolwide positive behavioral interventions and supports. Preventing School Failure, 60(1), 60-69. doi:10.1080/1045988X.2014.976809

Gennetian, L. A., Marti, M., Kennedy, J. L., Kim, J. H., & Duch, H. (2019). Supporting parent engagement in a school readiness program: Experimental evidence applying insights from behavioral economics. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 62, 1–10. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.12.006

Girardelli, D., & Patel, V. K. (2016). The Theory of Planned Behavior and Chinese ESL students' in-class participation. Journal of Language Teaching & Research, 7(1), 31-41. doi:10.17507/jltr.0701.04

Han, M., & Osterling, K. L. (2012). Characteristics and factors impacting reunification outcomes among Vietnamese immigrant families in the child welfare system. Children and Youth Services Review, 34103-111. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.09.005

Hilado, A. V., Kallemeyn, L., & Phillips, L. (2013). Examining understandings of parent involvement in early childhood programs. Early Childhood Research & Practice, 15(2), 1-9. Retrieved from http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v15n2/hilado.html

Hindman, A. H., Miller, A. L., Froyen, L. C., & Skibbe, L. E. (2012). A portrait of family involvement during Head Start: Nature, extent, and predictors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 654-667. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.002

Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Sandler, H. M., Walker, J. M. (2002). Parent Involvement Project (PIP) Parent and Teacher Questionnaires: Study 2. Department of Psychology and Human Development. Vanderbilt University, TN 37203.

Jeon, S., Choi, J. Y., Horm, D. M., & Castle, S. (2018). Early Head Start dosage: The role of parent-caregiver relationships and family involvement. Children and Youth Services Review, 93, 291–300. https://doi-org.proxy1.ncu.edu/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.07.032

Kikas, E., Tulviste, T., & Peets, K. (2014). Socialization values and parenting practices as predictors of parental involvement in their children's educational process. Early Education & Development, 25(1), 1-18. doi:10.1080/10409289.2013.780503

Kim, S. W., & Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: A meta-analysis of parental involvement and students’ academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(4), 919-934. doi:10.1037/edu0000023

Kiriakidis, S. (2015). Theory of planned behavior: The intention-behavior relationship and the perceived behavioral control (PBC) relationship with intention and behavior. International Journal of Strategic Innovative Marketing, 03, 40-51. doi.10.15556/IJSIM.02.03.004

Krogstad J. M. (2015). What Americans, Europeans think of immigrants. Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/09/24/what-americans-europeans-think-of-immigrants/

Lee, J., & Zhou, M. (2014). The success frame and achievement paradox: The costs and consequences for Asian Americans. Race & Social Problems, 6(1), 38. doi:10.1007/s12552-014-9112-7

Lin C. (2012). The effects of policy knowledge on attitudes and behaviors towards participation in educational policy-making among parents: A structural equation modeling approach. Education, 132(3), 1-15. Retrieved from https://www.questia.com/library/journal/1G1-283945584/the-effects-of-policy-knowledge-on-attitudes-and-behaviors

Manz, P. H., Gernhart, A. L., Bracaliello, C. B., Pressimone, V. J., & Eisenberg, R. A. (2014). Preliminary development of the parent involvement in early learning scale for low-income families enrolled in a child-development-focused home visiting program. Journal of Early Intervention, 36(3), 171-191. doi:10.1177/1053815115573077

McCormick, M. P., Cappella, E., O'Connor, E. E., & McClowry, S. G. (2013). Parent involvement, emotional support, and behavior problems: An ecological approach. Elementary School Journal, 114(2), 277-300.

McGregor, S. A., & Knoll, M. A. (2015). The Theory of Planned Behavior as a framework for understanding parental experiences with homework. Educational Psychology in Practice, 31(4), 335. doi:10.1080/02667363.2015.1065472

Miller, E. B., Farkas, G., Vandell, D. L., & Duncan, G. J. (2014). Do the effects of Head Start varying by parental pre-academic stimulation? Child Development, 85(4), 1385-1400. doi:10.1111/cdev.12233

Multiple regression analysis using SPSS Statistics (n.d.). Laerd Statistics. Retrieved from https://statistics.laerd.com/spss-tutorials/multiple-regression-using-spss-statistics.php

Ntuli, E., Nyarambi, A. and Traore, M. (2014), Assessment in early childhood education: threats and challenges to effective assessment of immigrant children. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 14: 221–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-3802.2012.01256.x

Paat, Y. (2013). Working with immigrant children and their families: An application of Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems theory. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 23(8), 954-966. doi:10.1080/10911359.2013.800007

Park, J., & Park, M. (2016). Qualitative versus quantitative research methods: Discovery or justification? Journal of Marketing Thought, 3(1), 1-7. doi: 10.15577/jmt.2016.03.01.1

Perry, A. R., & Langley, C. (2013). Even with the best of intentions: Paternal involvement and the theory of planned behavior. Family Process, 52(2), 179-192. doi:10.1111/famp.12013

Porumbu, D., & Necşoi, D. V. (2013). The relationship between parental involvement/attitude and children's school achievements. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 76(5), 706-710. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.04.191

Poureslami, I., Nimmon, L., Ng, K., Cho, S., Foster, S., & Hertzman, C. (2013). Bridging immigrants and refugees with early childhood development services: partnership research in the development of an effective service model. Early Child Development & Care, 183(12), 1924. doi:10.1080/03004430.2013.763252

Pratt, M. E., Lipscomb, S. T., & Schmitt, S. A. (2015). The effect of Head Start on parenting outcomes for children living in non-parental care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(10), 2944-2956. doi:10.1007/s10826-014-0098-y

Sibley, E., & Brabeck, K. (2017). Latino immigrant students' school experiences in the United States: The importance of family-school-community collaborations. School Community Journal27(1), 137-157. Retrieved from http://www.schoolcommunitynetwork.org/SCJ.aspx

Smith, S. C. (2014). Parental engagement in a Reggio Emilia inspired Head Start program. Early Childhood Research & Practice, 16(1). Retrieved from https://experts.illinois.edu/en/publications/parental-engagement-in-a-reggio-emilia-inspired-head-start-program

Smith, T. (2015). Qualitative and quantitative research. Research Starters: Education (Online Edition). Retrieved from http://proxy1.ncu.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ers&AN=89164394&site=eds-live

Tipton, J. A. (2014). Using the theory of planned behavior to understand caregivers’ intention to serve sugar-sweetened beverages to non-Hispanic black preschoolers. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 29(7), 564-575. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2014.07.006

Winsler, A., Kim, Y. K., & Richard, E. R. (2014). Socio-emotional skills, behavior problems, and Spanish competence predict the acquisition of English among English language learners in poverty. Developmental Psychology, 50(9), 2242-2254. doi:10.1037/a0037161